Ecstasy
from a Physiological Point of
View
By
KAJ BJÖRKQVIST
(Reproduced
here by courtesy of the author).
Religious
ecstasy can be studied from as many points of view as there
are disciplines in the scientific study of religion. It can
be studied, for example, from a sociological,
phenomenological, historical or a psychological point of
view. But there is also a physiological aspect to the
experience of ecstasy; and a physiological study of ecstasy
might bring to light new facts that would add to our general
knowledge of the phenomenon and perhaps lead to a better
understanding of it from other points of view, too. The very
close relationship between psychology and physiology can
hardly be questioned. The biological study of man is one of
today's most rapidly advancing sciences. There is no reason
for not utilizing these methodologies of research and the
knowledge already gained when studying ecstasy and other
similar religious phenomena. It is not my opinion that
ecstasy should be solely or even principally studied in a
physiological context; the shortcomings of such a
reductionistic viewpoint should be obvious, and have been
pointed out, for example, by Staal (1975, ch. 8). To the
comments of Staal, I would like to add that sociological
factors, like the social role of the person experiencing
ecstasy, and cognitive factors, like the interpretation of
the experience, are two variables that undoubtedly will have
a great effect when the experience of ecstasy is shaped in
the human consciousness. But physiological data,
meaningfully integrated with other bodies of data, will no
doubt broaden our understanding of the phenomenon of
ecstasy.
In
everyday language, the word "ecstasy" denotes an intense,
euphoric experience. For obvious reasons, it is rarely used
in a scientific context; it is a concept that is extremely
hard to define. Ecstasy-seeking religious movements have
always existed, and they seem today to be as numerous as
ever. There is perhaps no reason yet to abandon the concept
entirely. I will not attempt to define the term ecstasy
here, but I will say that I consider ecstasy, mysticism and
trance to be partly overlapping concepts. In mystical
experience, there is always an element of ecstasy, although
the presence of this element is not, in itself, enough to
justify calling an experience mystical. In trance, there is
often, but not necessarily, an element of ecstasy. In the
literature, trance has nonetheless often been used almost
synonymously with ecstasy. Furthermore, an experience can be
ecstatic without being either mystical or trance-like. An
experience can also be ecstatic without having any religious
connotation whatsoever. Since the borderline is so hard to
draw, I will in this discourse consider not only clear cases
of ecstasy, but also phenomena that are closely related to
it.
Something
should also be said about the body-mind-issue, the relation
between psyche and soma-an age-old problem in
philosophy. There are almost as many theories about their
relationship as there are philosophers. The materialists
stress soma at the total cost of psyche; the idealists do
the opposite. Of dualism, there are many different versions,
for instance parallelism, which proposes that the two
coexist without directly affecting each other. Without
discussing any of these further, I will simply state that I
here regard body and mind as two sides of the same
coin; the same phenomenon seen either from the point of
internal subjective experience, or from the point of
external objective observation. When a change occurs in
soma, a simultaneous change also occurs in psyche, and vice
versa. There cannot be a change in one without a
simultaneous change in the other. In the act of thinking,
neural activity takes place in the brain. Although we do not
know what a person is thinking by studying his brain waves,
we do know that different types of thinking result in
different types of brain waves (alpha and beta waves,
etc.).
The
claim that there cannot be a change in psyche without a
simultaneous change in soma is of course hard to prove,
since our instruments are not, and perhaps never will be,
sufficiently sensitive to register all the subtle changes in
the human nervous system. But in my opinion it is the most
plausible viewpoint, judging from the present scientific
data. Whether one wants to call this dualism or materialism
is a matter of taste. But this soma-psyche relationship is
the basic assumption which I will use as the starting point
for this discussion.
Since
mental states and physiological correlates always accompany
each other, it is obvious that the human mind can be
affected by external means, for instance by drugs. But the
opposite is also true; mental changes affect the body, as
they do in the case of psychosomatic diseases.
Aldous
Huxley was the first one to suggest that mystical
experiences are directly related to physiological changes
(Huxley, 1969; first published in 1954). He based his
hypothesis mainly on his own experiments with mescalin.
Huxley believed that adrenochrome, which is a product of a
decomposition of adrenalin and has a chemical composition
resembling that of mescalin, might be the cause of so called
"spontaneous" mystical experiences, since this substance, he
thought, might occur spontaneously in the human body in
sufficient amounts (Huxley, ibid., 12). He also
suggested that mystical experiences could be achieved by
means other than exogenous or endogenous hallucinogenics.
The breathing exercises of yogis, for example, might
increase the amount of CO2 in their lungs and blood, and
hence also in the brain (Huxley, ibid., 113). When
the ascetics of medieval times used to beat themselves
repeatedly, histamine was released into the blood, which
according to Huxley (ibid., 120) might have caused an
intoxication reminiscent of that achieved with
hallucinogenic drugs. He explained the prevalence of
mystical experiences in medieval times in a similar fashion:
it could have been the result of severe lack of vitamins
(Huxley, ibid., 1 17-1 19).
In
his proposals as to how the different ecstasy techniques
work physiologically, Huxley was probably often wrong (a
point to which I shall return). Modern research data have
refuted many of his hypotheses, or at least rendered them
unlikely. All this does not make Huxley less important; he
started a new paradigm in the psychology of religion. We
certainly have to agree with Huxley that physiological
variables must be taken into account when we ponder the
question of why religious practitioners all over the world
have used such similar ecstasy techniques. They have altered
their physical constitution by fasting or adhering to
vegetarianism; they have changed their hormonal balance by
living in celibacy; sometimes they have bombarded their
nervous system with drugs. At times the physiological
implications are more subtle and not so easily apprehended,
as in the case of meditation, prayer and the use of music.
The music might be slow and "soothing" such as one may hear
in a Christian mass or in a Tibetan temple, or it can be
fast and rhythmic, often combined with drumming and dancing,
as in voodoo. Meditation, prayer and music are undoubtedly
psychological rather than physiological, but studies of
brain waves have made it clear that they also have
physiological implications.
Drugs
have been used in all parts of the world as an ecstasy
technique (Furst, 1972). How the hallucinogenics work,
however, is not quite clear. Most researchers agree that
they seem to interfere with the transmitter substances in
the nervous system. The adrenochrome hypothesis, suggested
by Huxley (1969; see also above), appears unlikely to be
true-there does not seem to be enough adrenochrome in the
human body to account for any radical effects. Instead it
has been thought that LSD might interfere with the action of
serotonin. In some situations LSD has been shown to act like
serotonin, and in others to be a powerful antagonist to it;
thus LSD might either facilitate or block the neurohumoral
action of serotonin in the brain (Barron et al.,
1971). But it is far from proved that the effects of LSD can
be explained this way. It may be that LSD creates an
imbalance among several different neurohumors. The question
is still not settled.
Even
less is known about how fasting and vegeterianism work.
There has been surprisingly little study of the
physiological effects of reducing food intake and adhering
to special diets, and even less of the psychological
effects. EEG recordings obtained from the participants in a
fasting march from Jyvaskyla to Helsinki in Finland three
years ago showed more alpha activity after the march than
before (Björkqvist, S.-E., 1980). This is a quite
natural result of the participants being tired, and of
course a combined result of both marching and fasting; in
what proportions is impossible to say.
That
celibacy can also be considered as a "physiological" ecstasy
technique has been almost overlooked by the psychologists of
religion. It has not, to my knowledge, been discussed at
length in the literature. The psychodynamic implications of
sublimating the libido (in Freudian terms) have been
discussed, but the physiological implications have been
neglected. Still it is a fact that the vast majority of all
widely-known mystics have been living in celibacy. In
medieval times, when monasteries were common in Europe,
mysticism flourished. When the number of monasteries
decreased, mysticism also declined. Celibacy was of course
only one factor of many, but nevertheless probably an
important one. In Hinduism, celibacy or brahmacarya
has always been a highly emphasized technique. How it works
can at the present only be guessed at. But it is evident
that celibacy must have a profound influence on hormonal
balance in the body.
Some
authors (e. g., Ornstein, 1972) have suggested that,
neurologically, religious experiences are a function of the
right cerebral hemisphere. From the work of Sperry and
Gazzaniga (Gazzaniga, 1971), among others, we know that the
two brain hemispheres have, in part, very different
functions. The left hemisphere is connected with the right
side of the body, the right hemisphere with the left side.
In most people, the left hemisphere is dominant-a good
example being righthandedness. During childhood, the two
brain halves develop partly different functions. For
instance, the area of cortex dealing with hearing is in the
left hemisphere connected with speech; the corresponding
area in the right hemisphere is connected with the musical
ear and the comprehension of music. Speech, logical thinking
and mathematics are governed largely by the left hemisphere
(Luria, 1978). The right hemisphere seems to be the more
intuitive, artistic half of the brain. However, to claim
that religious experience is governed by the right
hemisphere would be to jump to conclusions.
Ecstasy
is often described as an extremely joyful experience; this
pleasure must necessarily also have a physiological basis.
It is of course too early to say anything for certain, but
the discovery of pleasure centres in the brain (e.g., Olds,
1971) might offer an explanation. These pleasure centres are
situated at different locations in the midline region of the
brain, but mostly in the hypothalamus. If an electrode is
inserted into the brain of an animal at a spot where a
pleasure centre is situated, and through a special
arrangement such as by stepping on a pedal the animal is
able to give itself electric stimulation in that specific
spot, something remarkable occurs; the pleasure is so
intense that the animal will forget everything. It will no
longer care about food or drink, and it will often simply
stimulate itself until it dies.
These
same pleasure centres also exist in the human brain. Could
it be that in the future we will find humans with electrodes
inserted into their brain, giving themselves electric
stimulation every now and then? A new, more horrible
addiction, worse than heroin? Let us hope this will not be
the case. But it is not far-fetched to suggest that when a
person experiences euphoric ecstasy, it might, in some way
or other, be connected with a cerebral pleasure
center.
The
best known studies of ecstatic experiences utilizing
physiological measurements have been the
electro-encephalographic (EEG) investigations of meditation.
A number of studies (e.g., Anand et al., 1961; Wenger
& Bagchi, 1961; Kasamatsu & Hirai, 1963, 1969;
Wallace, 1973; Banquet, 1973) have shown that the most
typical finding during meditation is a general slowing down
of the brain waves. Slow alpha waves with increased
amplitudes, sometimes even still slower theta waves (which
usually appear only during light sleep) are frequent during
meditation, regardless of the type-yoga, zen, or TM. In one
study, faster beta waves were more frequent (Das &
Gastaut, 1955) but in this study the subjects used a special
technique, Krya yoga, which involves strong
concentration on the point between the eyebrows with the
eyes crossed. Strong concentration is always correlated with
beta waves in the EEG. And even in this study, the yogis in
question had abundant alpha activity after finishing
meditation. Another observation (Banquet, 1973) is that the
deeper the meditation, the more synchronized this activity
seems to be, so that the same type of activity takes place
simultaneously all over the cortex. The neurons seem to fire
off in the same rhythm, regardless of where they are
located.
Alpha
waves are usually associated with a relaxed and positive
mood. Other physiological measurements have corroborated
this. During meditation, skin resistance is higher, which is
a sign of released tensions. The amount of lactate in the
blood decreases, which is also a sign of reduced tension
(Wallace, 1973). Heart rate and breathing slow down. Taken
as a whole, the entire metabolism slows down, with the
person remaining in a state of peaceful rest.
The
question can be raised: How is it possible that by
practising meditation a person can learn to increase, at
will, the amount of alpha activity in his brain? Although
alpha activity also appears in untrained subjects when they
are relaxed and have their eyes closed, it has been shown
(Wallace, 1973; Kasamatsu & Hirai, 1963, 1969) that the
better subjects are trained in meditation, the more frequent
and slower is their alpha activity; theta activity, which is
still slower than alpha, generally appears only among
trained meditators.
Functionally,
man has two nervous systems: one through which he can
voluntarily control certain muscles of the body, and another
called the visceral or autonomic nervous system (ANS).
Through the ANS are governed the activity of the heart, the
stomach and other internal functions. It has been thought
that we are unable to control these voluntarily to even the
slightest degree, but this has lately been shown to be
false. Miller (1971) was the first to demonstrate that
learning in the ANS can take place at least to some degree.
For instance, through a feedback mechanism, subjects,
whether they are animals or humans, can to a slight
degree learn to slow down or speed up their heart beat. This
phenomenon has been called biofeedback. Biofeedback can also
be applied to the activity of the brain. It has been shown
that people can learn to increase their alpha activity
(e.g., Kamiya, 1969) or even their theta activity (Green,
1975) at will. In this learning process the person sits with
electrodes attached to his head, through which brain wave
activity is registered. Whenever alpha activity appears in
the EEG, a special sound automatically registers through a
loudspeaker; and when the alpha disappears, quite another
sound is aired. In that way, the subject can acquire a
"sense" for how alpha activity feels, and is slowly able to
increase it at will. This can also have a therapeutic
effect, reminiscent of that of meditation (Green,
1975).
The
experiments with brain wave feedback give a hint of how
meditation works. In a way, meditation can be seen as a kind
of biofeedback without the alpha activity monitored out
through speakers; instead, the meditator listens to subtle
inner signals. To help in this process he can use one of
several meditation techniques which differ slightly
depending on the religious tradition or group he belongs
to.
In
India, breathing exercises have been used as an ecstasy
technique. But also in zen, the counting of inhalations or
exhalations has been used as a meditation technique
(Kapleau, 1970, 32). As mentioned earlier, Huxley thought
that breathing exercises could cause religious experiences
by increasing the amount of CO2 in the brain. This is highly
unlikely-breathing CO2 is a very unpleasant experience,
which anyone knows who has been in a room with extremely bad
air.
There
is another possible explanation, which l have suggested in
an earlier work (Björkqvist, K., 1976). Breathing is an
exceptional function in that it is controlled both by the
ANS and the somatic or voluntary nervous system. Some
breathing occurs automatically all the time, and we cannot
at will stop breathing altogether. The ANS sees to that. But
we can voluntarily change the rhythm as well as the depth of
our breathing. Moreover, breathing is closely related to a
number of functions governed solely by the ANS, such as
heart beat, perspiration, and various mechanisms related to
emotions. As a consequence, breath control or simply being
attentive to one's breathing would probably be the easiest
biofeedback technique to learn, since it does not involve
any direct or difficult learning in the ANS.
If
meditation increases alpha activity, then listening to slow,
soothing religious music, like Christian hymns or Tibetan
temple music, can be expected to have a somewhat similar
effect: a slowing down of the brain waves. But how can we
then explain that the complete opposite-fast, rhythmic music
combined with drumming and dancing-has been a popular
ecstasy technique in many cultures?
Pavlov
(1928), in his conditioning experiments with dogs, had
already observed something which he called the
ultraparadoxical phase. Both when the dogs were totally
overstimulated and when in certain stages of drowsiness,
there occurred a distortion of the effects of the
conditioning stimuli. The positive stimuli lost their
effect, and the negative became positive. Sargant (1951,
1957) suggested that something similar could be the case in
man when sudden political or religious conversion occurred.
He interpreted this phenomenon, which he called
"transmarginal inhibition", brought on by these
transmarginal stimuli (=stimuli exceeding the limit), as
some kind of collapse in the reactiveness after intense
mental tension or excitement.
This
has direct relevance in the study, for instance, of the
physiological effect of voodoo dancing. Oswald (1959) found
to his surprise that when he repeatedly gave strong electric
shocks to his subjects, they fell asleep! He also found
(Oswald, ibid.) that the signs of sleep in the EEG
could repeatedly come and go in rhythm with regular stimuli
at intervals of only a few seconds. When people were dancing
to the rhythm of a popular dance band, they could actually
fall asleep for a few seconds while still moving. In another
experiment (Oswald, 1960), he taped the eyelids of his
subjects so that they were unable to close their eyes. He
then gave them electric shocks and bright visual stimulation
to the rhythm of a blues band. All his subjects fell
asleep.
Voodoo
dancing, and similar practises, can easily be understood in
terms of the inhibition caused by transmarginal stimulation.
All the time they are dancing, dancers are on the border
between sleep and wakefulness, at times actually sleeping
while dancing. It is easy to understand that visions can be
seen in such a state. Sundén (1967) has interpreted
the experiences of zen monks in similar terms to Sargant and
Oswald.
The
spiritual practice of prayer, on the other hand, seems to
have a lot in common with meditation. Neither prayer nor
meditation are in themselves necessarily ecstatic
experiences; but they are practices during which ecstasy
frequently occurs. It is therefore reasonable to consider
them as ecstasy techniques. The effect of prayer on the body
has not been studied very much with physiological methods.
When Mallory (1977) used EEG in her investigation of monks
and nuns at prayer, she found no increase in alpha activity
during prayer compared with the activity recorded in a
control situation-. She did find, however, that extraverted
subjects had more abundant alpha activity than others, and
that the abundance of alpha correlated positively with
contemplation and negatively with neuroticism.
Synchronization of alpha activity was another interesting
point; it was reduced when subjects had difficulties in
contemplating. Contemplation was thus associated with both
alpha abundance and alpha synchronization. Mallory, summing
up her results, says that subjects who were good at
contemplation tended to have the same degree of alpha
abundance as experienced yoga or zen practitioners. The only
possible difference was that the alpha activity of her
subjects remained the same in the control situation, sitting
with closed eyes, but not praying, as in the experimental
situation, sitting with closed eyes, contemplating. Having
abundant alpha activity seemed to be the natural state for
these subjects when relaxed (Mallory, 1977).
In
a study in progress, I have been measuring EEG, GSR (skin
conductance and skin potential), heart beat and eye
movements (EOG) of ten persons while they pray. All belong
to a sect calling themselves "rukoilevaiset" (The Prayers),
whose members live in South West Finland. The members of
this sect pray regularly and often.
The
subjects were first told to sit still with their eyes
closed, and simply to tune themselves in on being close to
God, but not actually to pray. This was called the
contemplation condition. They were then told to pray as they
normally did, either with words or silently, whichever they
preferred.
Some
typical results are seen in figures 1-6. Two records are
shown for three subjects.
During
the contemplation condition, alpha activity was common among
most subjects. A typical case is shown in fig. 1. One
subject (fig. 3) even had very slow theta
activity.
|